On recognizing learning disorders: where do we stand?
![]() |
Photo by agu²! on Foter.com / CC BY-NC-SA |
In schools I work with
what I see are isolated efforts from practitioners to understand what is
happening inside the brains of students who are genuinely struggling to learn.
On one side, medical doctors and psychology practitioners are clad in the
protective realm of their knowledge of the brain and mind, respectively,
whereas the empirical knowledge and the evidence derived from the classroom
remains largely ignored by diagnoses that fail to listen to the accounts and
informed opinions of teachers.
The fact that each
brain is unique and uniquely organized (Devlin & Poldrack, 2007) and that
much will be shaped at pre-natal stages (Aamodt & Wang, 2012) should
make it easy for teachers, on the other side, to avoid subscribing to methods
and approaches that promise learning based on fads and myths (Dekker,
Howard-Jones & Jolles, 2012). What astonishes me is how quickly we all
cling to short-cuts in understanding something so complex as the brain,
avoiding taking the hard yet necessary route of systemic learning (Toga &
Thompson, 2007). The fact that we do prefer the use of heuristics (our
System1 according to Kahneman & Egan, 2011) on everyday tasks should be
discussed and broadly divulged wherever there is a genuine quest for knowing
more about different learning and developmental trajectories.
Whereas there is
nothing wrong in acknowledging what we all do, the fact that we have to remain
vigilant – and humble, disciplined and effortful – in our quest to process
information differently when complex issues are considered should be widely
known and attempted. That is, in my view, the way to assimilate information
about the developmental stages, learning thresholds and differences. It takes a
collective effort to respect the individuality of a human being in a learning
trajectory that has to be supported by that very same community. We should all,
for instance, strive to furnish each individual with quality of input and
interaction as it does impact their language acquisition and, consequently,
their learning trajectory (Saxton, 2017). We may still lie in our infancy in
terms of knowledge about the brain, but baby steps in the right direction
should be a joint effort for all in Education to take.
And you, where do you stand in recognizing that we all learn in different ways?
References
Aamodt, S.& Wang, S. (2011). Welcome to your child’s brain: How the mind grows from conception to college. New York: Bloomsbury
Craik, F. I., & Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of
processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of verbal learning and
verbal behavior, 11(6), 671-684.
Dekker, S., Lee, N. C., Howard-Jones,
P., & Jolles, J. (2012). Neuromyths in education: Prevalence and predictors
of misconceptions among teachers. Frontiers in psychology, 3, 429.
Devlin, J. T., & Poldrack, R. A.
(2007). In praise of tedious anatomy. Neuroimage, 37(4),
1033-1041.
Green, C. S., & Bavelier, D. (2008).
Exercising your brain: a review of human brain plasticity and training-induced
learning. Psychology and aging, 23(4), 692.
Kahneman, D., & Egan, P. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow (Vol. 1). New York: Farrar,
Straus and Giroux.
Lockhart, R. S., & Craik, F. I.
(1990). Levels of processing: A retrospective commentary on a framework for
memory research. Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue
canadienne de psychologie, 44(1), 87.
Saxton, M. (2017). Child language: Acquisition and development (2nd
ed.). Los Angeles, CA: SAGE.
Toga,
A. W., & Thompson, P. M. (2007). What
is where and why it is important. Neuroimage, 37(4),
1045.
UCD (21 Sept. 2009). “Ulysses Medal for
Groundbreaking Memory Scientist.” University College Dublin,
UCD News. Accessed at www.ucd.ie/news/2009/09SEP09/210909_kandell.html.
Comments
Post a Comment